THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 2021, VOL. 161, NO. 4, 466–476 https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2021.1883503 Cyberbullying perpetration in the COVID-19 era: An application of general strain theory Christopher P. Barlett, Alexis Rinker, and Brendan Roth Gettysburg College ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY The world is currently grappling with the medical, psychological, economic, and behavioral consequences of the COVID-19 global pandemic. The existing research has rightly been focused on the medical contributions – treatment, symptoms, prevalence, etc. – which are paramount. A paucity of research has tested the psychological and behavioral consequences of COVID-19. In two cross-sectional studies of US adults, we posited that personal (e.g., being diagnosed with COVID-19) and proximal (e.g., knowing people with COVID19) experiences with COVID-19 would be related to cyberbullying perpetra­ tion due to an increase in stress. Using path modeling, results showed that (a) personal and proximal COVID-19 experiences positively correlated with cyberbullying (Studies 1 and 2) and (b) personal COVID-19 experiences were indirectly related to cyberbullying through stress, but not proximal experiences (Study 2). Received 1 July 2020 Accepted 14 January 2021 KEYWORDS COVID-19; cyberbullying; stress; general strain theory Cyberbullying – repeatedly and purposefully harming others through electronic mediums (Englander et al., 2017) – has emerged as an important societal issue across the industrialized world – for children and adults alike. Understanding the variables and psychological processes involved in predicting cyberbullying is paramount for prevention and intervention efforts. Recent myriad positive strides have been made in these research endeavors. Indeed, much scholarship has been devoted to developing and validating cyberbullying intervention programs (e.g., Gaffney et al., 2019), quality meta-analytic findings have elucidated the variables germane to cyberbullying (e.g., Kowalski et al., 2014), and theory has continued to be developed and refined to explain why and for whom cyberbullying is likely (c.f., Barlett, 2019). Despite such progress, global and societal-level changes often present challenges in predicting cyberbullying perpetration. The waxing and waning of social networking platforms, changes to (cyber)bullying laws, instances of social injustice, and others may present unintended difficulties in predicting cyberbullying perpetration by possibly changing the strength and/or direction of existing cyberbullying relationships. Currently, the world is experiencing the health, economic, psychological, and sociological consequences of the 2019 novel coronavirus (SARS-cov-2; here in referred to as COVID-19). The World Health Organization declared COVID-19 an international public emergency on January 30, 2020 and, as the current writing of this paper (11/4/20), there are currently over 47 million cases worldwide (World Health Organization (WHO), 2020) with over 9 million cases in the United States alone (CDC, 2020a). The current research presents two studies aimed at understanding how COVID-19 has impacted cyberbullying perpetration in a sample of US adults. There have been numerous academic papers written about the coronavirus – predominantly from medical researchers – that largely addresses important issues surrounding this virus, such as pre­ valence rates (Ceylan, 2020), treatment (Sanders et al., 2020), antibody testing (Xiang et al., 2020), CONTACT Christopher P. Barlett cbarlett@gettysburg.edu Washington St., Gettysburg, PA, 17325 © 2021 Taylor & Francis Department of Psychology, Gettysburg College 300 N. THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 467 lifetime of the virus (Lippi et al., 2020), symptomatology (Sohrabi et al., 2020), who is at risk (CDC, 2020b), and others. Social scientists – psychology included – have an equally important, albeit different, role in the accumulation of coronavirus knowledge – testing the psychological and beha­ vioral predictors and outcomes associated with the experiences of COVID-19. Given the timing of the virus, it is unsurprising that a paucity of social scientific research exists, but some findings are emerging. Indeed, research has shown psychological impacts of COVID-19 including stress, depres­ sion, and anxiety (Wang et al., 2020), and citizen engagement (Chen et al., 2020). We believe that an accumulated knowledge of COVID-19 effects across multiple disciplines is germane, and the current research focuses on understanding the behavioral outcomes associated with COVID-19 experiences and the psychological mediators that explain such effects. We chose to study the relationship between COVID-19 experiences (e.g., being diagnosed and/or being in close proximity with infected others) and cyberbullying perpetration. Although some could argue that cyberbullying perpetration is not as important to study as other behaviors, we contend that understanding the processes that predict antisocial online behavior due to a global pandemic is imperative. A potential fallout from COVID-19 is the likely increase in stress caused by imminent unexpected life changes, and cyberbullying perpetration may be how some individuals react to said stress. Recently, Conway et al. (2020) created and validated various scales that purportedly measure the personal (e.g., “I have been diagnosed with coronavirus [COVID-19]”) and proximal (e.g., “I know someone who has had coronavirus-like symptoms in the past four months”) experiences with COVID19. The measurement of these constructs enables social psychologists to study whether, why, and for whom COVID-19 experiences predict behavior, such as cyberbullying. In Study 1 we test if COVID-19 experiences correlate with cyberbullying perpetration, and, if such relationships are discovered, then we test if stress was an indirect cause in these relationships in Study 2. General strain theory We hypothesized an indirect relationship between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying through stress. Indeed, myriad stressors due to COVID-19 have influenced people across the world. For instance, schools across the world have closed and students are completing their academic year from home putting new pressure on parents/guardians. Also, there is an increased likelihood of job loss, being furloughed, or working remotely which likely increases stress. Theoretically, the General Strain Theory (GST; Agnew, 1992) can explain such relationships. GST posits that stressors (or strains) may either (a) prevent, or threaten to prevent, people from attaining positively valued goals, (b) reduce the likelihood of access to positive stimuli, and/or (c) increase negative affect due to exposure to negative stimuli (Agnew, 1992). Stressors, such as those explicated by GST, have been shown to predict broader aggressive behaviors (Berkowitz, 1988). Indeed, one common reaction to the presence of a strain is anger and aggression with the goal of eliminating the strain (Agnew, 1992). GST has been applied to the study of cyberbullying. Patchin and Hinduja (2011) showed a positive relationship between the number of strains (e.g., disagreement with family, money problems, being victimized, etc.) and cyberbullying. Similarly, Lianos and McGrath (2018) showed that academic and financial strains juxtaposed with prior cyberbullying experiences predicted later cyberbullying perpe­ tration; a finding conceptually replicated by Paez (2018) with social strains. In short, GST posits that the presence of strains (COVID-19 experiences in our study) predicts stress (our proposed mediator) to yield antisocial behavior (cyberbullying perpetration). Overview of the current research The primary objective of our research is to examine the direct relationship between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration. Two correlational studies sampling US adults from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk tested these relationships. Study 1 examined the simple relationships between COVID-19 proximal and personal experiences and cyberbullying perpetration. Study 2 468 C. BARLETT ET AL. examined the same relationships; however, we also tested the mediating role of stress. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk was an ideal location to recruit participants for several reasons. First, research has shown that cyberbullying frequency is higher in adulthood than youth (Barlett & Chamberlin, 2017). Mechanical Turk only allows adults to be sampled. Second, to date COVID-19 has impacted partici­ pants across the 50 US states differently. Mechanical Turk samples people across the US allowing for variability in COVID-19 experiences, which is important for our statistical analyses. The data for both studies are available upon request from the corresponding author. The data are not readily available due to ethical constraints. Study 1 The purpose of Study 1 was to examine the correlations between proximal COVID-19 experiences, personal COVID-19 experiences, and cyberbullying perpetration. We hypothesize positive correla­ tions between these variables. Personal COVID-19 experiences, according to Conway et al. (2020), refer to whether the participant her/himself has been diagnosed with COVID-19 and/or had symp­ toms of COVID-19, etc. Proximate experiences, in contrast, refer to whether or not close others to the participant have had COVID-19 symptoms or diagnoses. These measures do not directly ask about the degree of symptoms (e.g., asymptomatic vs. severe symptoms), number of diagnoses, or the relation­ ship between the participant and the others close to them who are sick (for proximate experiences). Although such extensions could be considered in future work and questionnaires, the current study used a reliable and valid measure of our constructs. Method Participants One hundred and ninety-four US participants (64.9% male) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. The average age of the sample was 37.15 (SD = 10.33) years. The majority of participants were White (69.6%). The only selection criterion in MTURK was to limit our sample to live in the US. Materials Materials for both Studies 1 and 2 are registered at https://osf.io/2jvgr. Cyberbullying perpetration The cyberbullying perpetration scale from the Cyberbullying Experiences Survey (Doane et al., 2013) instructs participants to report how frequently they have engaged in various cyberbullying behaviors using a 1 (never) to 6 (everyday/almost everyday) rating scale. A sample item is “Have you sent a rude message to someone electronically?” The 25 items were summed such that higher scores indicate higher levels of cyberbullying perpetration (α = .99). We modified this scale by asking about the frequency of the behaviors in the past three months, rather than the past year, in order to ensure that the cyberbullying behaviors occurred concurrently with COVID-19. COVID-19 experiences The Coronavirus Experiences Questionnaire (Conway et al., 2020) was modified for the purposes of the current study. This is a 10-item measure that asks participants about their experiences with the coronavirus using two subscales. The first subscale is the three-item Personal Experience (α = .79) subscale (e.g., “I have been diagnosed with coronavirus”) and the second subscale is the four-item Proximal Experience (α = .74) subscale (e.g., “I know someone who has had coronavirus-like symptoms in the last three months”). We modified the measure to have the response options be in THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 469 a yes/no format (coded as 1 = yes, 0 = no). Certain items were reverse coded and summed, such that higher scores indicate higher personal or proximate experiences with COVID-19. The remaining three items assessed news exposure, which were not analyzed in the current study. Demographics Participants were asked about their age, gender, and ethnicity. Income, education level, socioeconomic status, and other demographic information were not ascertained. We also included an attention check question to track which participants dutifully completed the study. This question asked which sports the participant played in high school, but also explained that some individuals do not fully attend to survey questions, then the question instructed participants to select “soccer” and “tennis”. Those who correctly answered the question were retained for our analyses. After excluding those who failed the attention check (10.8%), the sample included 173 participants (64.2% male) and the average age of the sample was 37.23 (SD = 10.10) years. The majority of participants were White (70.5%). There was no difference in age, t(192) = 0.30, p = .768, or sex X2(1) = .43, p = .510, between the included and excluded participants. Procedure Institutional review board approval was granted for this study. Data was collected as part of a larger study.1 US participants were recruited using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. The data were collected in one day in April 2020. Participants completed the online informed consent before completing the aforementioned questionnaires. Participants were then thanked, fully debriefed, and compensated ($1US) for their time. Results Correlations Table 1 displays the parametric (Pearson) and non-parametric (Spearman Rank-Ordered) correlations between the variables of interest. Of theoretical interest, results showed that cyberbullying perpetra­ tion was related to personal (r = .64, p < .001) and proximal COVID-19 experiences (r = .56, p < .001). Table 1 also shows that all the variables were significantly not normal (e.g., skewed and/or kurtotic). Sex differences Due to research showing sex differences in cyberbullying (Barlett & Coyne, 2014), we used both parametric (independent samples t-test) and non-parametric (Mann-Whitney U) tests to examine sex differences on the variables of interest to check if we needed to statistically control for such variation. Table 1. Correlations between relevant variables in study 1. Variable 1: Cyberbullying 2: COVID-19 experiences (personal) 3: COVID-19 experiences (proximity) M SD Shapiro-Wilk Skewness (Z) Kurtosis (Z) 1 – 64*** .56*** 56.63 33.19 .83*** .79 −4.45*** 2 .58*** – .69*** .93 1.17 .73*** 4.05*** −2.76** 3 .56*** .65*** – 1.52 1.45 .83*** 2.00* 3.24** Numbers below the diagonal are Pearson correlation coefficients and numbers above the diagonal are Spearman rank-ordered correlation coefficients. * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 470 C. BARLETT ET AL. Table 2. Sex differences in key variables in study. Variable Cyberbullying COVID personal COVID proximity Male Mean (SD) 56.70 (33.40) .95 (1.20) 1.56 (1.47) Female Mean (SD) 56.52 (33.10) .90 (1.11) 1.44 (1.42) t .03 .22 .53 Z −.52 −.03 −.52 Results are displayed in Table 2 and showed that males and females responded similarly on all variables. Therefore, sex of participant was not treated as a covariate in our primary analyses. Multiple regression We tested whether personal and proximal COVID-19 experiences predicted cyberbullying using a multiple regression. Results showed that the model accounted for a significant proportion of variances in cyberbullying perpetration, R2 = .44, F(2,155) = 60.40, p < .001. Examination of the unique effects showed that both personal COVID-19 experiences (B = 14.34, SE = 2.43, t(155) = 5.98, p < .001) and proximal COVID-19 experiences (B = 4.64, SE = 1.94, t(155) = 2.39, p = .018) predicted cyberbullying perpetration. In both cases, as experiences with COVID-19 increased, cyberbullying increased. Discussion Results from Study 1 showed that COVID-19 experiences (both personal and proximal) were correlated and predicted cyberbullying perpetration. This suggests that when individuals have more personal experiences with the COVID-19 pandemic or know close others who have been infected with COVID-19 their likelihood of cyberbullying others increases. Study 2 Since the results from Study 1 showed the COVID-19 experiences predicted cyberbullying perpetra­ tion in US adults, Study 2 was conducted to (a) replicate the correlations uncovered in Study 1 and (b) test the mediating effect of perceived stress in the relationships between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration. The former rationale is especially important since different regions were having very different COVID infection rates, COVID-related restrictions, and COVID-related online debates that might invite cyberbullying in April 2020 (when data from Study 1 was collected). We collected data over one month later where more of the US was entrenched in COVID-19 effects to see if the findings from Study 1 replicated. Method Participants One hundred and eighty-eight US participants (65.6% male) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. The average age of the sample was 37.96 (SD = 11.29) years. The majority of participants were White (80.70%). The only selection criterion in MTURK was to limit our sample to live in the US.2 Materials The same cyberbullying perpetration (α = .99), COVID-19 experiences (α = .79), and demographic questionnaire from Study 1 were used in Study 2. In addition, we adapted the Perceived Stress Scale (S. THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 471 Cohen et al., 1983) to assess stress. We adapted the measure by changing the time predicate of each item from “last month” to “last five months” in order to ensure that we were assessing stress during COVID-19 in the US and to temporally match the cyberbullying perpetration measure. This is a 10item questionnaire that asks participants to rate their level of agreement with the items on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) rating scale. A sample item is “In the last 5 months, I have you been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly?” Certain items were reverse coded before sum­ ming, such that higher scores indicate higher levels of perceived stress (α = .80). Finally, analysis of the attention check item on the demographic questionnaire revealed that 23 (12%) of the sample were removed from the analyses. Thus, the final sample consisted of 169 (65.7% male) participants with an average age of 37.38 (SD = 11.04) and the majority White (81.7%). There was no difference in age, t (186) = 1.90, p = .059, or sex, X2(1) = .00, p = .965, between the included and excluded participants. Procedure Institutional review board approval was granted for this study. Data was collected as part of a larger study.3 US participants were recruited using Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. The data were collected in one day in June 2020. Participants completed the online informed consent before completing the aforementioned questionnaires. Participants were then thanked, fully debriefed, and compensated ($2US) for their time. Results Correlations Table 3 displays the parametric (Pearson) and non-parametric (Spearman Rank-Ordered) correlations between the variables of interest. Of theoretical interest, results showed that cyberbullying perpetra­ tion was related to personal (r = .56, p < .001) and proximal COVID-19 experiences (r = .40, p < .001) and perceived stress (r = .44, p < .001). Moreover, perceived stress correlated with personal (r = .33, p < .001) and proximal (r = .32, p < .001) COVID-19 experiences. Finally, both types of COVID-10 experiences were correlated (r = .66, p < .001). Sex differences Replicating Study 1, parametric (independent samples t-test) and non-parametric (Mann-Whitney U) tests showed that males and females responded similarly on all variables. Therefore, sex of participant was not treated as a covariate in our primary analyses (see Table 4). Table 3. Correlations between relevant variables in study 2. Variable 1: Cyberbullying 2: COVID-19 experiences (personal) 3: COVID-19 experiences (proximity) 4: Perceived Stress M SD Shapiro-Wilk Skewness (Z) Kurtosis (Z) 1 – .56*** .40*** .44*** 48.68 30.73 .81*** 3.00** −3.39*** 2 .51*** – 66*** .33*** .99 1.15 .77*** 3.42*** 3.05*** 3 .43*** .65*** – .32*** 1.62 1.53 .83*** 1.53 −3.58*** 4 .49*** .32*** .32*** – 27.77 7.08 .96*** −2.26* 1.16 Numbers below the diagonal are Pearson correlation coefficients and numbers above the diagonal are Spearman rankordered correlation coefficients. * p < .05; ** p < .01; ** p < .001 472 C. BARLETT ET AL. Table 4. Sex differences in key variables in study 2. Variable Cyberbullying COVID personal COVID proximity Stress Male Mean (SD) 50.01 (29.28) .97 (1.12) 1.72 (1.53) 27.95 (6.83) Female Mean (SD) 46.18 (33.43) 1.03 (1.23) 1.45 (1.51) 27.42 (7.61) t .76 −.33 1.08 .46 Z −1.34 −.16 −1.12 −.09 1.15 (.89 to 1.17) Proximal COVID-19 Experiences Personal COVID-19 Experiences 1.38 (.21 to 2.55) .75 (-.27 to 1.72) Perceived Stress .23 (-3.17 to 3.66) 12.05 (7.30 to 16.57) 1.24 (.63 to 1.94) Cyberbullying Perpetration Figure 1. Path modeling results with stress in study 2. Dashed lines are not significant. Path modeling We tested the relationships between COVID-19 experiences, perceived stress, and cyberbully­ ing perpetration using path modeling procedures in MPLUS with maximum likelihood estima­ tion. Due to the skewed nature of the data, we used 5000 bootstrapped samples to estimate unstandardized regression coefficients with 95% confidence intervals. We tested the postulates of the General Strain Model by examining whether stress was the indirect cause of the effect between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration; our model first had COVID19 personal and proximity subscales correlated and predicting perceived stress that predicted cyberbullying perpetration. Finally, COVID-19 personal and proximal experiences predicted cyberbullying perpetration. Consistent with Study 1, all paths and relationships were estimated leaving no degrees of freedom to estimate model fit, and, thus, the data was a perfect fit for the data. Figure 1 displays the unstandardized path coefficients with 95% confidence intervals. Results showed that perceived stress and personal COVID-19 experiences predicted cyberbullying per­ petration. Moreover, personal COVID-19 experiences predicted perceived stress. Proximal COVID-19 experiences correlated with personal COVID-19 experiences, but did not predict stress or cyberbullying perpetration. INDIRECT model statements were added to test the mediating effects between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration through stress. Consistent with the previous analysis, stress was the indirect causal variable in the relationship between personal COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration (Indirect B = 1.71, 95% CI: .26 to 3.60), but not proximal COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration (Indirect B = .93, 95% CI: −.32 to 2.43). THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 473 Discussion Replicating the results of Study 1, results from Study 2 showed that personal and proximal COVID-19 experiences correlated with cyberbullying perpetration. Building upon these effects, Study 2 showed that perceived stress was the mediating variable in the effect of COVID-19 personal experiences and cyberbullying perpetration. Specifically, as COVID-19 personal experiences increased stress also increased to yield higher levels of cyberbullying perpetration. General discussion The COVID-19 global pandemic has undoubtedly changed everyday life for billions of people across the globe. The majority of countries, states, and territories have “locked down” their citizens in order to reduce the spread of COVID-19. Schools (daycares, primary, secondary, and colleges/universities) have closed and moved to mostly remote instruction, offices have shifted from corporate buildings to employee residences, jobs have been lost or furloughed, and restaurants have moved exclusively to drive thru or carryout only (for those that stayed in business). The psychological and behavioral impact of these, and other, consequences of COVID-19 are not yet well understood; however, such understanding is imperative for informing parents, spouses, children, counselors, clinicians, and others how COVID-19 has affected people. The current research examined the direct (Studies 1 and 2) and indirect relationship (Study 2) between personal and proximal experiences with the novel coronavirus, stress, and cyberbullying perpetration in a sample of US adults. First, we wanted to understand if COVID-19 experiences would correlate with cyberbullying perpetration. Results from both studies showed that as personal and proximal experiences with the virus increased so did the frequency of cyberbullying perpetration. In other words, the more experiences people have with COVID-19 the more frequent cyber-attacks are committed. Since we found reliable relationships between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration, Study 2 was conducted in order to understand whether stress indirectly causes these relationships. Stress was chosen because (a) antidotal evidence suggests that COVID-19 has caused much stress and (b) according to the General Strain Theory (GST; Agnew, 1992), strains (or stressors) can result in delinquent behaviors, including cyberbullying (e.g., Paez, 2018; Patchin & Hinduja, 2011). Our results suggest that stress does, indeed, act as the indirect variable in the relationship between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration. Interestingly, this indirect effect only occurred for personal – not proximate – COVID-19 experiences. This suggests that perceived stress positively predicts cyber­ bullying perpetration only if the novel coronavirus has personally affected an individual, but not their friends or acquaintances. Although post-hoc, such findings may appear intuitive. One’s own personal diagnoses with COVID-19 will likely increase the worry and stress regarding their own health and livelihood, and, in accordance with GST, one way to express or relieve that stress is to harm others online. While this study mainly intended to simply discover the direct and indirect relationships between COVID-19 experiences and cyberbullying perpetration from a theoretical point of view, we wanted to explicate the practical implications. Given the paucity of research examining how COVID-19 experi­ ences yield psychological and behavioral changes, we wanted to better understand how cyberbullying perpetration is related to such experiences in order to better inform parents, counselors, clinicians, school administrators, and others about such effects. For instance, findings from our study suggest that those who encounter and/or attempt to combat cyberbullying and cyber-victimization should target reducing the impact of stress – as a function of COVID-19 personal experiences. If no measures are taken to alleviate stress caused by COVID-19, then there will likely be more stress and cyberbullying. Limitations and future research Although the current research yielded interesting direct and indirect results, we view this as an important first step that can inform future studies. Like all psychological studies, there are some 474 C. BARLETT ET AL. limitations that need to be addressed and studied in future research. First, the data is correlational and causal claims cannot be made. True, we tested a causal model in our primary analyses, based on theory and past work; however, other research designs should be tested in future work. For instance, researchers can compare cyberbullying perpetration and levels of stress from 2019 (before COVID19 was rampant in the US) to one year later. Another option is to longitudinally examine cyberbullying across time to see how cyberbullying perpetration changes with COVID-19 experiences. Indeed, as more people become infected with the virus, it may be interesting to see how cyberbullying perpetra­ tion changes. Second, our data collection and analysis only presents the relationships between COVID-19 experiences, stress, and cyberbullying perpetration at a single point in time. The importance of our findings, therefore, is to demonstrate that under the conditions at the time of data collection in the US, cyberbullying was predicted by COVID-19 proximal and personal experiences and stress. Third, General Strain Theory (Agnew, 1992) posits many different types of strains that can influence behavior, and findings in the cyberbullying literature shows that strains other than work and family stressors can cause cyberbullying (e.g., Patchin & Hinduja, 2011). We specified our measurement of strains to be specific to COVID-19 experiences. Future work should attempt to include other types of stressors or strains that changed as a function of COVID-19 experiences, such as: feelings of isolation, the difficulty in getting resources from local grocery stores or markets, and others. Fourth, our procedures called for the use of self-reported measures of antisocial behaviors (cyber­ bullying perpetration) from a Mechanical Turk sample. Although we have previously argued that for this project, a Mechanical Turk sample was ideal; however, varying beliefs, attitudes, past behaviors, living situations, etc. are commonplace when Mechanical Turk users are sampled. We diligently removed participants from our analyses who were not adequately paying attention; however, webbased psychological research with such a heterogeneous sample is not as ideal as sampling a more homogeneous participants. Although these differences often are conceptualized in the error term in our analyses, future work should attempt to replicate our results with either a more homogeneous sample or statistically address such variation. Related, we relied on self-report methods. Currently, in the cyberbullying literature, there are no other valid procedures for measuring cyberbullying perpe­ tration (Barlett, 2019); however, future work should attempt to validate our methods using alternative measures. Finally, generalizations about our findings to samples beyond US adults are ill-advised. We elected to sample adults, rather than adolescents, for our study for two reasons. First, the strain items we created are not appropriate for youth because the items inquire about work and family stressors. We do acknowledge that youth are likely experiencing the deleterious psychological effects of COVID-19 experiences akin to adults, and future work should test whether cyberbullying perpetration is due to stressors relevant to you. Second, research has shown that cyberbullying peaks in adulthood, rather than in adolescence (Barlett & Chamberlin, 2017). Thus, we wanted to sample individuals who are most likely to cyberbully others in an effort to provide information to those who can help reduce cyberbullying. Notes 1. Other trait questionnaires were measured, but not analyzed in this study. These included, COVID-19 concerns and impact (Conway et al., 2020), anonymity perceptions (Wright, 2013), Belief in the Irrelevance of Muscularity for Online Bullying (Barlett & Gentile, 2012), cyberbullying attitudes (Barlett et al., 2016), cyber-victimization (Doane et al., 2013), and various other demographic questionnaires (e.g., number of kids, relationship status, time spent online, etc.). We do not believe that the inclusion of these questionnaires would alter the results and there were no experimental manipulations that would have interfered with answers on these measures. 2. A power analysis was conducted using G*Power to test if the sample size used had adequate power. For Study 1, we derived our effect size from the reported correlation between COVID personal and proximate experiences from Conway et al. (2020; r = .58) and opted for power of .95, with a two-tailed alpha of .05. Results showed that THE JOURNAL OF SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY 475 we needed 28 total participants. In Study 2, we used the same the correlation between cyberbullying and COVID experiences reported in Study 1 (r = .56) and results showed that 31 total participants were necessary to achieve optimal power. In short, both studies are adequately powered. 3. Other trait questionnaires were measured, but not analyzed in this study. These included, the Dirty Dozen (Jonason & Webster, 2010), questions related to quarantine experiences, the Ten Item Personality Inventory (Gosling et al., 2003), impulsivity (Steinberg et al., 2013), cyber-aggression (Runions et al., 2017), guilt (T. R. Cohen et al., 2011), and various other demographic questionnaires (e.g., number of kids, relationship status, time spent online, etc.). We do not believe that the inclusion of these questionnaires would alter the results and there were no experimental manipulations that would have interfered with answers on these measures. Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Notes on contributors Dr. Christopher P. Barlett is an associate professor in the psychology department at Gettysburg College. He studies the predictors and consequences of cyberbullying perpetration. Alexis Rinker is currently an undergraduate student at Gettysburg College. She is a psychology major and has worked in Dr. Barlett's Aggression Research Lab for over a year. Brendan Roth is currently an undergraduate student at Gettysburg College. He is a psychology major and has worked in Dr. Barlett's Aggression Research Lab for over a year. Data availability statement The data described in this article are openly available in the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/j78sc/. Open scholarship This article has earned the Center for Open Science badge for Open Materials. The materials are openly accessible at https://osf.io/j78sc/. References Agnew, R. (1992). Foundation for a general strain theory of crime and delinquency. Criminology, 30(1), 47–87. https:// doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1992.tb01093.x Barlett, C. P. (2019). Predicting cyberbullying: Research, theory, and intervention. Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/c20180-00531-9 Barlett, C. P., & Chamberlin, K. (2017). Examining cyberbullying across the lifespan. Computers in Human Behavior, 71, 444–449. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.009 Barlett, C. P., & Coyne, S. M. (2014). A meta-analysis of sex differences in cyber-bullying behavior: The moderating role of age. Aggressive Behavior, 40(5), 474–488. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21555 Barlett, C. P., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Attacking others online: The formation of cyberbullying in late adolescence. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1(2), 123–135. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028113 Barlett, C. P., Helmstetter, K., & Gentile, D. A. (2016). The development of a new cyberbullying attitude measure. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 906–913. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.013 Berkowitz, L. (1988). Frustrations, appraisals, and aversively stimulated aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 14(1), 3–11. https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-2337(1988)14:1<3::AID-AB2480140103>3.0.CO;2-F Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020a, May 22). Cases in the U.S. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019ncov/cases-updates/cases-in-us.html Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020b). People who need extra precautions. https://www.cdc.gov/corona virus/2019-ncov/need-extra-precautions/index.html 476 C. BARLETT ET AL. Ceylan, Z. (2020). Estimation of COVID-19 prevalence in Italy, Spain, and France. Science of the Total Environment, 729, 138817. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2020.138817 Chen, Q., Min, C., Zhang, W., Wang, G., Ma, X., & Evans, R. (2020). Unpacking the black box: How to promote citizen engagement through government social media during the COVID-19 crisis. Computers in Human Behavior, 110, 106380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106380 Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), 385–396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404 Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 947–966. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0022641 Conway, L. G., III, Woodard, S. R., & Zubrod, A. (2020). Social psychological measurements of Covid-19: Coronavirus perceived threat, government response, impacts, and experiences questionnaires. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ z2x9a Doane, A. N., Kelley, M. L., Chiang, E. S., & Padilla, M. A. (2013). Development of the cyberbullying experiences survey. Emerging Adulthood, 1(3), 207–218. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167696813479584 Englander, E., Donnerstein, E., Kowalski, R., Lin, C. A., & Parti, K. (2017). Defining cyberbullying. Pediatrics, 140(2), 148–151. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758U Gaffney, H., Farrington, D. P., Espelage, D. L., & Ttofi, M. M. (2019). Are cyberbullying intervention and prevention programs effective? A systematic and meta-analytical review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 45, 134–153. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2018.07.002 Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(6), 504–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1 Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 420–432. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019265 Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., & Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychological Bulletin, 140(4), 1073–1137. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0035618 Lianos, H., & McGrath, A. (2018). Can the general theory of crime and general strain theory explain cyberbullying perpetration? Crime & Delinquency, 64(5), 674–700. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011128717714204 Lippi, G., Sanchis-Gomar, F., & Henry, B. M. (2020). Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): The portrait of a perfect storm. Annals of Translational Medicine, 8(7), 497. https://doi.org/10.21037/atm.2020.03.157 Paez, G. R. (2018). Cyberbullying among adolescents: A general strain theory perspective. Journal of School Violence, 17 (1), 74–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/15388220.2016.1220317 Patchin, J. W., & Hinduja, S. (2011). Traditional and nontraditional bullying among youth: A test of general strain theory. Youth and Society, 43(2), 727–751. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X10366951 Runions, K. C., Bak, M., & Shaw, T. (2017). Disentangling functions of online aggression: The cyber-aggression typology questionnaire (CATQ). Aggressive Behavior, 43(1), 74–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21663 Sanders, J. M., Monogue, M. L., Jodlowski, T. Z., & Cutrell, J. B. (2020). Pharmacologic treatments for coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): A review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 323(18), 1824–1836. https://doi. org/10.1001/jama.2020.6019 Sohrabi, C., Alsafi, Z., O’Neill, N., Khan, M., Kerwan, A., Al-Jabir, A., Iosifidis, C., & Agha, R. (2020). World health organization declares global emergency: A review of the 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID-19). International Journal of Surgery, 76, 71–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijsu.2020.02.034 Steinberg, L., Sharp, C., Stanford, M. S., & Tharp, A. T. (2013). New tricks for an old measure: The development of the barratt impulsiveness scale–brief (BIS-brief). Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 216–226. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0030550 Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(5), 1729–1753. https:// doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729 World Health Organization. (2020, May 22). WHO Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) dashboard. https://covid19.who.int Wright, M. F. (2013). The relationship between young adults‘ beliefs about anonymity and subsequent cyber aggression. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16(12), 858–862. https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2013.0009 Xiang, F., Wang, X., He, X., Peng, Z., Yang, B., Zhang, J., Zhou, Q., Ye, H., Ma, Y., Li, H., Wei, X., Cai, P., & Ma, W. (2020). Antibody detection and dynamic characteristics in patients with COVID- 19. Clinical Infectious Diseases, 22, 1930–1934. https://doi.org/10.1093/cid/ciaa461